• Welcome to the new Internet Infidels Discussion Board, formerly Talk Freethought.

The Science of 'Truthiness'

rousseau

Contributor
Joined
Jun 23, 2010
Messages
13,508
http://www.washingtonpost.com/poste...logy-explains-why-people-are-so-easily-duped/

True or false: “The Eiffel Tower is in France.” Most of us can quickly and accurately answer this question by relying on our general knowledge. But what if you were asked to consider the claim: “The beehive is a building in New Zealand.” Unless you have visited New Zealand or watched a documentary on the country, this is probably a difficult question. So instead of recruiting your general knowledge to answer the claim, you’ll turn to your intuition. Put another way, you’ll rely on what Stephen Colbert calls “truthiness” — truth that comes from the gut, and not books.

As a cognitive psychologist, I study the ways that memory and belief go awry: How do we come to believe that things are true when they are not? How can we remember things that never actually happened? I am especially intrigued by the concept of truthiness — how smart, sophisticated people use unrelated information to decide whether something is true or not.

For instance, in a classic study by Norbert Schwarz and Rolf Reber at the University of Michigan, people were more likely to think a statement was true when it was written in high color contrast (blue words on white) as opposed to low contrast (yellow words on white). Of course, the color contrast has nothing to do with whether the claim is true, but it nonetheless biased people’s responses. The high color contrast produced a feeling of truthiness in part because those statements felt easier to read than the low color contrast statements. And it turns out that this feeling of easy processing (or low cognitive effort) brings with it a feeling of familiarity. When things feel easy to process, they feel trustworthy — we like them and think they are true.

In my research at UC Irvine, I have collaborated with psychologists in New Zealand and Canada to discover the ways we can be tricked into thinking thst something feels familiar, trustworthy and true. In our studies, we have focused on how photos and names can have surprisingly powerful effects on our memories, beliefs and evaluations of others.

Photographs can boost comprehension and make it easier for us to learn and remember new information. But cognitive psychology research shows that photos can also have an insidious influence — they can lead us to believe and remember things are true when they are not. In a study by Elizabeth Loftus and others at UC Irvine, people who saw a doctored photo of President Obama shaking hands with the former Iranian president Mahmoud Ahmadinejad actually said they remembered the event happening — even though it was completely false. Photos can even trick us into remembering false events from our own childhood. People who saw a doctored childhood photo came to remember a false event (riding in a hot air balloon) with the same detail and emotion that you would expect from a real memory.

Photos are a record of real events, so it’s not surprising that we often view them as the best evidence that something actually happened. What is more surprising is our recent work showing that photos can alter our beliefs even when they do not provide any evidence for the claim at hand. In a study we conducted in New Zealand at Victoria University of Wellington, we found that when people read a statement (such as “Macadamia nuts are in the same evolutionary family as peaches”) alongside a decorative photo that simply related to the claim (a bowl of macadamia nuts), they were more likely to believe that the claim was true. That is, these decorative photos produced truthiness — photos that were related to but did not depict the claim encouraged people to believe that the claims were credible. Moreover, this truthiness effect persisted over days, not minutes, and could have long-lasting effects on people’s beliefs.

But visual cues are not the only source of non-diagnostic evidence that people use to evaluate claims. People can be influenced by even more subtle features of information, like the linguistic attributes of a word.

We know that pronunciation can influence our judgments about products, stocks and activities. Put simply, people prefer things that are easy to pronounce. We think that Magnalroxate is a safer food additive than Hnegripitrom. We think that the roller coaster called Ohanzee is less risky than the one called Tsiischili. And in the stock market, easy-to-pronounce ticker codes (KAR) perform better than their difficult-to-pronounce counterparts (RDO) — even after just one day of trading.

It is one thing for pronunciation to influence perceptions of products, amusement park rides and stocks. Surely we don’t let such an irrelevant cue influence our ideas about another person?

But it turns out that we do. People who have easier-to-pronounce names are thought to be safer, less risky and more familiar. We give them more votes than their counterparts with difficult-to-pronounce names. We even use the pronunciation of a person’s name as a source of information to evaluate the credibility of his or her claims. In our most recent study, we asked people to evaluate the truth of a series of statements — half were attributed to someone with an easy-to-pronounce name, and half were attributed to someone with a difficult-to-pronounce name. We found that when the claims were paired with easy-to-pronounce names, people were more likely to think they were true. People believed the claim “Turtles are deaf” more when it was attributed to “Andrian Babeshko” than when it was attributed to “Czeslaw Ratynska.” The easy names produced truthiness.

Of course, the pronunciation of a name or a loosely related photograph should have no influence on people’s judgment of truth. So why do they influence our judgments? Like the high-color contrast statements, claims attributed to those with an easy name or those accompanied by a photo feel easier to process. The easy names require less cognitive effort; a photo helps people to visualize and understand a claim more rapidly. This feeling of easy processing is often taken as a sign that information is familiar, credible and true. To the Fred Flintstone parts of our brains, that feeling of familiarity signals something that we can trust, while information that’s difficult to process signals danger.

This feeling of familiarity could influence us in a variety of contexts. In the courtroom, an easy name might make a witness or expert seem more credible. In the workforce, an easy name might help an individual’s résumé float to the top of a stack. And in the news, a photo — even one that is only loosely related — might make a story seem more credible.
Games - Click Here for More!

So how can we avoid being taken in by a false sense of truthiness? Cognitive psychology research has shown that people are often unaware of their biases or how information influences their judgments. But simply being warned about the influence of names and photos might just make us a little more cautious — leading us to look for truth that comes from books, and not the gut.

Turns out things as simple as the complexity of people's names lead people to bias.
 
People believed the claim “Turtles are deaf” more when it was attributed to “Andrian Babeshko” than when it was attributed to “Czeslaw Ratynska.”

But that actually seems rational. Should we really be getting our scientific claims from Polaks?

On a serious note, this is good research, but I wish they would NOT use vague concepts like "intuition". Most of their examples are really more about diffuse emotional or mood states being used to make an evaluation of something that is not the source of the mood. Hard to say words can evoke a subtle negative mood state, and we are more likely to evaluate something in a way that has the same positive or negative valence as our current mood. A claim being false or a stock being risky are generally negative attributes, so if our mood is negative due to any unrelated reason we use that mood as a subtle cue that the thing we are evaluating has negative attributes. This is pretty old stuff, consistent with research from 40 years ago called "Misattribution of arousal" in which physiological states of arousal, get interpreted as emotions, which get misattributed to things in the environment, such people in a mild state of arousal after a strenuous activity misattributing their arousal as sexual attraction to an opposite sex person they are talking to.

The problem with calling this "intuition" is that intuition need not be emotion based inferences, but can be information based, pooling a lifetime of relevant empirical experiences into a vague sense of the correct answer. It can mean you actually heard the correct answer before but you cannot retrieve the episodic memory of when you heard it so you just have a vague sense of what the right answer is. Or maybe the information is indirect, such as "What is the Capitol of France?" Even if you have never heard the correct answer, if you just guess that a city name that you've heard often, you are more likely to be correct then guessing a city name you never hear. You didn't really know the answer, and in a sense you went with your "gut", but it isn't emotion and its the kind of "gut" that is really your mind attending to frequency patterns in the environment and giving rise to a sense of familiarity that is likely to covary with objective attributes of things.
 
That explains a lot.

My first name is a palindrome while my second is four syllables. I commonly go by my second name because that is what my parents always addressed me as. This rather explains why people either like me or hate me, no middle ground. :D

Interestingly, even my detractors trust me.

They may not like me, but they trust me.

My two names, when spoken together, are quite melodic, and roughly translate in meaning to 'gracious in victory.'
 
Read Popper's Objective Knowledge An Evolutionary Approach.

The only objective knowledge is that which is empirically demonstrated.

How the masses come to accept something as truth is a complex social process.

The heroic myth of Custer vs his actions. The belief that the BB is an absolute truth.

The popular uses and misinterpretations of quantum physics.

I do not see how it is a revelation. Mass marketing-propaganda makes use of it. Political skill is in creating 'thuthiness'.
 
I do not see how it is a revelation.

It isn't supposed to be a "revelation". It's science, which 99% of time clarifies and specifies what we know rather than instantly reveals something completely outside of our knowledge.

Mass marketing-propaganda makes use of it. Political skill is in creating 'thuthiness'.

If by "it" you mean the vague and general notion that people's beliefs are irrational and impacted by things other than empirical data, then yes. However, the specific finding of this research that the mere difficulty in pronouncing a word can, entirely on its own, have notable causal impacts on several different evaluative judgments for which the word is a rather meaningless label is something we did not know before this research. The fact that it is not a very surprising finding doesn't mean we "knew it all along", because many false claims also don't seem surprising. Also, note the the research in question is not hot off the presses, but some of it is decades old.
 
That explains a lot.

My first name is a palindrome while my second is four syllables. I commonly go by my second name because that is what my parents always addressed me as. This rather explains why people either like me or hate me, no middle ground. :D

Interestingly, even my detractors trust me.

They may not like me, but they trust me.

My two names, when spoken together, are quite melodic, and roughly translate in meaning to 'gracious in victory.'

Why am I thinking of Anna Karenina?
 
That explains a lot.

My first name is a palindrome while my second is four syllables. I commonly go by my second name because that is what my parents always addressed me as. This rather explains why people either like me or hate me, no middle ground. :D

Interestingly, even my detractors trust me.

They may not like me, but they trust me.

My two names, when spoken together, are quite melodic, and roughly translate in meaning to 'gracious in victory.'

Why am I thinking of Anna Karenina?

Dunno...

I was thinking Anna Victoria.
 
You guys are thinking of an annagram, not a palindrome. Duh!

Anna, the first name is a palindrome, and they've got that part correct. I must have been tired when I posted, because my second name is actually only three syllables, lol. :eek:

There is probably a four syllable word for my lapse of attention to such details. :D;)
 
Back
Top Bottom